fbpx

Juliana Osmani


University “Aleksandër Moisiu” of Durrës, Faculty of Business, Management Department; Address: L.9, Rr: “AleksandërGoga”, P. 979/2, Durrës, Albania

2nd International Scientific Conference on Recent Advances in Information Technology, Tourism, Economics, Management and Agriculture – ITEMA 2018 – Graz, Austria, November 8, 2018, CONFERENCE PROCEEDINGS published by the Association of Economists and Managers of the Balkans, Belgrade, Serbia; ISBN 978-86-80194-13-4

Abstract

The overconfidence of the decision maker is necessary to achieve good results and could inspire in others a sense of trust, but overconfidence can become an obstacle to decision-making process. So, it can create problems for the organization as for example can damage the organization value, can lead to the loss of competitive advantages, reduction of the market share and incomes, weak tendency to innovation, too slow responses to changes, inaccurate risk evaluation, increased risk to bankruptcy etc. If overconfidence is accompanied by excessive optimism, it may become even more dangerous because the decision maker is confident that his abilities are better than those of others, but he is also convinced that the future will be favorable. Decisions taken under these conditions can have bad consequences, both in the economic and non-material aspect.

The main goal of the research is to understand if there is a correlation between overconfidence and age. 210 managers have participated in the study. The intention is to identify variables and elements and to discover possible relationships between them, so as to be able to define some general reflections about the age impact on overconfidence. Starting from previous studies, which show a decline of cognitive and decisional abilities as age advances, it has been hypothesized that the correlation between age and overconfidence is negative. The processing and analysis of the collected data indicate that there is no correlation between these two variables. Thus, overconfidence is independent of age.

Key words

decision-making, age, overconfidence, cognitive abilities, decision-making skills


References

[1] Mariani M. (2009), Decidere e negoziare, Il Sole 24 ORE, Milano

[2]   Bazerman M.H., Moore D.A. (2009), Judgment in managerial decision making, 7th edition, New York: John Wiley & Sons, Inc.

[3]   Rosenberg M. (1965), Society and the adolescent self-image, Princeton, NY: Princeton University Press

[4]   Devolder P.A. (1993), Adult age differences in monitoring of practical problem-solving performance. In Hershey D.A., Wilson J.A. (1997), op. cit.

[5]   Pliske R.M., Mutter S.A. (1996), Age differences in the accuracy of confidence judgments. In Rolison J.J., Hanoch Y., Wood S. (2012), Risky decision making in younger and older adults: The role of learning, Psychology and Aging, Vol. 27, No. 1, pp. 129-140

[6]   Crawford J., Stankov L. (1996), Age differences in the realism of confidence judgments: A calibration study using tests of fluid and crystallized intelligence, Learning and Individual Differences, Vol. 6, pp. 84-103

[7]   Hershey D.A., Wilson J.A. (1997), Age differences in performance awareness on a complex financial decision-making task, Experimental Aging Research, Vol. 23, pp. 257-273

[8]   Mienaltowski A. (2011), Everyday problem solving across the adult life span: solution diversity and efficacy, Annals of the New York Academy Sciences, Vol. 1235, pp. 75-85

[9]   Finucane M.L., Gullion C.M. (2010), Developing a tool for measuring the decision making competence of older adults, Psychology and Aging, Vol. 25, No. 2, pp. 271-288

[10] Berg C.A., Meegan S., Klaczynski P.A. (1999), Age and experiential differences in strategy generation and information requests for solving everyday problems, International Journal of Behavioral Development, Vol. 23, pp. 615-639

[11] Payne J.W., Bettman J.R., Schkade D.A. (1999), Measuring constructed preferences: Towards a building code, Journal of Risk and Uncertainty, Vol. 19, pp. 243-270

[12] Healey M.K., Hasher L. (2009), Limitations to the deficit attenuation hypothesis: Aging and decision making, Journal of Consumer Psychology, Vol. 19, No. 1, pp. 17-22

[13] Reed A.E., Mikels J.A., Simon K.I. (2009), Older adults prefer less choice than young adults, Psychology and Aging, Vol. 23, No. 3, pp. 671-675

[14] Riggle E.D.E., Johnson M.M.S. (1996), Age differences in political decision making: Strategies for evaluating political candidates, Political Behavior, Vol. 18, No. 1, pp. 99-118

[15] Mather M. (2006), A review of decision making processes: Weighing the risks and benefits of aging. In Carstensen L.L., Hartel C.R. (Eds.), When I’m 64, pp. 145–173, Washington, DC: National Academies Press

[16] Henninger D.E., Madden D.J., Huettel S.A. (2010), Processing speed and memory mediate age related differences in decision making, Psychology and Aging, Vol. 25, No. 2, pp. 262-270

[17] Wood S., Busemeyer J., Koling A., Cox C.R., Davis H. (2005), Older adults as adaptive decision makers: Evidence from the Iowa Gambling Task, Psychology and Aging, Vol. 20, pp. 220-225

[18] Glück J., Bluck S. (2011), Laypeople’s conceptions of wisdom and its development: Cognitive and integrative views, The Journals of Gerontology, Vol. 66, No. 3, pp. 321-324

[19] Artistico D., Cervone D., Pezzuti L. (2003), Perceived self-efficacy and everyday problem solving among young and older adults, Psychology and Aging, Vol. 18, No. 1, pp. 68-79

[20] Thornton W.J.L., Dumke H. (2005), Everyday problem solving and decision making in aging: a meta-analytic review, Psychology and aging, Vol. 20, No. 1, pp. 85-99

[21] Patrick J.M.H. (1995), Aging and expertise effects on decision making processes and outcomes. In Thornton W.J.L., Dumke H.A. (2005), op. cit.

[22] Morrow D.G., Menard W.E., Stine-Morrow E.A., Teller T., Bryant D. (2001), The influence of expertise and task factors on age differences in pilot communication, Psychology and Aging, Vol. 16, pp. 31-46

[23] Hertwig R., Barron G., Weber E.U., Erev I. (2004), Decisions from experience and the effect of rare events in risky choice, Psychological Science, Vol. 15, No. 8, pp. 534-539

[24]     Cervone D., Artistico D., Berry J. (2006), Self-efficacy processes in adult development. In Hoare C.H. (Eds.), The Oxford handbook of adult development and learning, pp. 167-195, Oxford University Press

osmani_decision_making_and_overconfidence_is_there_a_correlation_with_age_pp_945-951

Connect with us

Association of Economists and Managers of the Balkans – UdEkoM Balkan
179 Ustanicka St, 11000 Belgrade, Serbia

https://www.udekom.org.rs/home

Udekom Balkans is a dynamic non-governmental and non-profit organization, established in 2014 with a mission to foster the growth of scientific knowledge within the Balkan region and beyond. Our primary objectives include advancing the fields of management and economics, as well as providing educational resources to our members and the wider public.

Who We Are: Our members include esteemed university professors from various scientific disciplines, postgraduate students, and experts from ministries, public administrations, private and public enterprises, multinational corporations, associations, and similar organizations.

Building Bridges Together: Over the course of ten years since our establishment, the Association of Economists and Managers of the Balkans has established impactful partnerships with more than 1,000 diverse institutions across the Balkans region and worldwide.

ITEMA conference publications are licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial 4.0 International License.